There is no doubt that our brain
is the source of intelligent behavior as was vividly illustrated by
Mountcastle's quote (1975, p. 131): “Each of us lives within the universe – the
prison – of his own brain. Projecting from it are millions of fragile sensory
nerve fibers, in groups uniquely adapted to sample the energetic states of the
world around us: heat, light, force and chemical composition. That is all we
ever know of it directly; all else is logical inference.”
This reminds me of the movie
Matrix starring Keanu Reeves as Neo; or Vanilla sky with Tom Cruise as David
Aames who, after his suicide, is placed in a lucid dream as part of a Life
Extension program.
More recently Robert Lanza in his
book Biocentrism (BenBella Books, Inc., 2009) argued that “custom has told us
that what we see is “out there,” outside ourselves, and such a viewpoint is
fine and necessary in terms of language and utility, as in “Please pass the
butter that’s over there.” But make no mistake: the visual image of that
butter, that is, the butter itself, actually exists only inside your brain. That
is its location. It is the only place visual images are perceived and cognized.”
He further suggests: “Some may
imagine that there are two worlds, one “out there” and a separate one being
cognized inside the skull. But the “two worlds” model is a myth. Only one
visual reality is extant, and there it is. Nothing is perceived except the
perceptions themselves, and nothing exists outside of consciousness. Only one
visual reality is extant, and there it is. Right there. The “outside world” is,
therefore, located within the brain or mind.” (Lanza and Berman, p.31.)
Therefore in our opinion only a
brain perspective on intelligence can bring us closer to a better understanding
of it. Let me explain.
Research into the neural
underpinning of intelligence has mainly adopted a construct perspective: trying to find structural and functional
brain characteristics that would accommodate the psychological construct of
intelligence. For example, Colom and colleagues
(2006) related brain structure, specifically gray matter volume, to the general
factor of intelligence (g). The
results of their study showed that performance on two prototypical measures of
general intelligence correlates with gray matter volume across frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital lobes.
A less common approach is to
investigate how the brain functions in order to see if this corresponds to the
construct of intelligence – the
brain perspective. The most far-reaching attempt in the so-called brain perspective is a study conducted
by Hampshire and colleagues (2012). The authors compared factor analyses of
brain imaging, behavioral, and simulated data in order to determine whether
different components of intelligence are paralleled by distinct brain networks.
Functional brain imaging data was analyzed within the Multiple Demand Cortex [1].
The activation patterns resulted in two principal components: a working memory
component (insula, the superior frontal sulcus, and the anterior cingulate cortex)
and a reasoning component (inferior frontal sulcus, inferior parietal cortex,
and parts of anterior cingulate cortex). See figure below. The analysis of
behavioral data resulted in 3 factors, two of which corresponded to the
components extracted based on brain activity, while the third included tasks
that used verbal information. According to the authors, these findings suggest
that intelligence is an emergent property of multiple specialized brain
systems, each of which has its own capacity (Hampshire et al., 2012).
[1] The multiple demand system refers to
frontal and parietal brain area activation patterns related to diverse
cognitive demands (for a review see Duncan, 2010).
In an exploratory study conducted
in our lab (Jaušovec and Jaušovec 2010), an even broader question was put
forward: is there a typology of neuro-electric brain activity that could
explain individual differences in human behavior? To answer the question an
analysis of electroencephalography data based on resting brain activity of 331
individuals was performed. A hierarchical cluster analysis revealed 3 neuro-clusters
which differed based on: (1) local and long range coding of information and (2)
the way the nervous system entrains to rhythmic environmental stimulation and
reacts when the environmental stimuli lack regularity. These differences
tentatively correspond to the switching between the default mode network [2] and the
central executive network [3].
The behavioral analysis showed
that the difference between the neuro-clusters was mainly due to individuals
scoring high on performance IQ, strategic emotional intelligence, and the
personality factors of conscientiousness and openness. This finding is similar
to the results reported by Hampshire and colleagues (2012) and further questions
the assumption that our brain organization supports a single g solution.
However, as recently pointed out
by Haier and colleagues (2014), so far none of the imaging studies have found
evidence for a “neuro-g” - yet this does not mean that there is none.
To our knowledge the only
definition of intelligence that completely relies on the central nervous system
was suggested by Jensen (2011; p. 173):
“Intelligence is the periodicity of neural oscillation in the action
potentials of the brain and central nervous system.” According to Jensen, action potentials are
the building blocks of all our thoughts, thus also of intelligence. Their
periodical fluctuation is a response to incoming stimuli. A simplified example
could be the change from alpha waves (8-12 Hz) when one’s eyes are closed to
fast oscillations like beta waves (20 Hz <) when one’s eyes are opened.
The bad news about the brain
approach at the moment is that it has some methodological difficulties. This
was also admonished by Hampshire and colleagues (2014; p. 17):
“[…] no neuroimaging method can
accurately measure the capacity of a functional brain network; consequently,
that approach for estimating ‘g’ is entirely intractable.”
References
Buckner,
R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R. and Schacter, D. L. (2008), The Brain's Default Network.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124: 1–38.
doi:10.1196/annals.1440.011
Colom,
R., Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2006). Distributed brain sites for the
g-factor of intelligence. Neuroimage, 31(3), 1359-1365. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.006
Duncan,
J. (2010). The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: mental
programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends in cognitive sciences, 14(4),
172-179. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004
Haier,
R. J., Karama, S., Colom, R., Jung, R., & Johnson, W. (2014). A comment on
“Fractionating Intelligence” and the peer review process. Intelligence, 46, 323–332. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2014.02.007
Hampshire, A., Highfield, R. R., Parkin, B. L., & Owen, A. M.
(2012). Fractionating Human Intelligence. Neuron, 76(6), 1225–1237. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.022
Hampshire, A., Parkin, B., Highfield, R., & Owen, A. M. (2014).
Brief response to Ashton and colleagues regarding Fractionating Human
Intelligence. Personality
and Individual Differences, 60, 16–17. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.11.013
Jaušovec, N. &
Jaušovec, K., (2010). A typology of human neuro-electric brain activity and its
relation to personality and ability, Ricerche
di Psichologia, 2; 291-306.
Jensen,
A. R. (2011). The theory of intelligence and its measurement. Intelligence, 39(4),
171–177. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2011.03.004
Lanza,
R., & Berman, B. (2010). Biocentrism:
How life and consciousness are the keys to understanding the true nature of the
universe. BenBella Books.
Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010).
Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function.
Brain Structure and Function, 214(5-6), 655-667. http://doi:10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0
Mountcastle,
V. B. (1975). The View from Within: Pathways to the Study of Perception. Johns
Hopkins Medical Journal, 136, 109–131.
[2]
The default mode network participates in internal modes of cognition and shows
deactivation during performance on demanding cognitive tasks. The main hubs of
this network are the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the posterior cingulate
cortex (Buckner et al., 2008).
[3] The central-executive
network is involved in maintenance and manipulation of information in working
memory and plays a role in goal-directed behavior and problem solving. Key areas
include the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and posterior parietal cortex (Menon
& Uddin, 2010).
Free from Herpes just in 2 weeks
ReplyDeleteThe best herbal remedy
You can contact him
r.buckler11 {{@gmail}} com,, .......