One could say: “Creativity,
yet another ill-defined construct – why bother?” In psychology, new
concepts are often created to explain old ones, although for none of them accepted
definitions exist. Such concepts are just “cheap
talk”, as dismissed by Deary (2000). However, creativity is a fundamental
human ability that has enriched our lives. Furthermore, it is related to
intelligence, therefore it deserves to be included in this blog. In our next posts
we are going to discuss the relationship between creativity and intelligence,
the process of illumination – insight, and the possibility of increasing creativity by means of interventions.
Modern creativity
research began with Guilford’s farewell address as president of the American
Psychological Association in 1950. In his article, Guilford (1950) called for
the study of creativity introducing the concept of divergent thinking – defined
as the ability to generate multiple solutions to an open-ended problem (Guilford,
1967). The theoretical frame was provided by Guilford’s structure-of-intellect morphological
model which, in the last version, comprises 180 different intellectual factors
organized along three dimensions: operations (cognition, memory recording,
memory retention, divergent production, convergent production and evaluation),
content (visual, auditory, symbolic, semantic and behavioral), and products
(units, relations, systems, transformations and implications). This model allows
the study of 30 different facets of divergent production (6 content characteristics
x 5 product types). The task Make a
Figure (factor loading 0.61 on divergent production of figural units – DFU) requires individuals to combine 2 line elements into different
figures in a great variety of ways (see Figure 1).
Figure
1.
Make a Figure test: Given two line
elements, the individual has to combine them in a great variety of ways to make
figures.
An example for the DMS factor (divergent production of
semantic systems) was the Four-Word Combinations FL test (DMS factor loading
0.59), which required the examinee to use 4 words with the same initial letters
in a number of different meaningful sentences (no word to be repeated):
Task: W________ C________ E________N________
Possible answers: “We can
eat nuts”, “Who colored Emma’s nose?”, “Why cannot elephants navigate?” “What
caused Eve’s nuisance?”
At that time also the first tests measuring divergent production appeared. Among the most established were the Torrance tests of creative thinking (TTCT), Guilford’s Alternate uses test, and Mednick’s test of remote associates. The common characteristic of all tests of divergent thinking was that they allowed for multiple answers (factor of fluency). The answers were then assessed according to originality or novelty and flexibility – the number of different types or categories of ideas. The main critique was that divergent thinking is not a prerequisite for creativity, because creativity can be also the result of convergent thinking, as demonstrated by the cases of Edison and his nearly algorithmic approach to inventing, or Bach’s methodical way of composing hundreds of cantatas. On the other hand, divergent thinking does not necessary yield creative products. It is further unclear whether or not creativity is psychometrically unitary as is the case with the g factor in intelligence. Hence, it is not surprising that neuroscience was not as interested in studying creativity as it was in other constructs such as intelligence or working memory. Nevertheless, in the last 15 years the number of studies investigating the neurobiological underpinnings of creativity has increased. A Web of Science search for the terms creativity and brain revealed 8 review papers or meta-analyses (Abraham, 2013; Arden, et al., 2010; Dietrich and Kanso, 2010; Gonen-Yaacovi, et al., 2013; Heilman, 2016; Pidgeon et al., 2016; Sawyer, 2011; Vartanian, 2012).
The first three meta-analyses on the neural underpinnings
of creative behavior showed a rather devastating picture with respect to
conclusions and answers provided. Dietrich and Kanso (2010) analyzed a total of
72 experiments, which were reported in 63 papers. They broadly classified them into 3
categories: divergent thinking, artistic creativity, and insight. The authors
concluded (Dietrich and Kanso, 2010, p. 822): “Taken together, creative thinking does not appear to critically depend
on any single mental process or brain region, and it is not especially
associated with right brains, defocused attention, low arousal, or alpha
synchronization, as sometimes hypothesized.” The only brain area that was
most often mentioned in relation to creative cognition was the prefrontal
cortex. However, the relation was complex. EEG studies revealed both increases
and decreases in power measures as well as in measures of synchrony. Similar
were the findings for neuroimaging studies based on the hemodynamic principle,
reporting both activations and deactivations.
The same conclusion was put forward by Arden and
colleagues (2010), who included 45 papers in their meta-analysis. The authors were critical about the fact that nearly as many tests and measures of creative cognition as the number of studies
included in the analysis were used. Thus, it is difficult to conclude if the differences in
brain activity should be attributed to
the process of divergent thinking, or to differences in tasks and neuroimaging
techniques employed. The suggestion put forward was that the focus should move
to basic psychometric work in developing more valid and reliable measures and
tests of creative cognition.
Sawyer (2011) in his review concluded that the entire
brain is active while individuals are engaged in creative thinking. Identified
were over twenty different brain regions related to the creative process. The
areas involved were the same as with other routinely accomplished every-day tasks
requiring no creativity. The critique was again directed toward the inconsistent operationalization of creativity
and the wide range of tasks used for its determination. Abraham (2013) therefore
suggested three main areas that should be improved in future neuroimaging research
of creative cognition:
- Optimizing neuroimaging paradigms, which includes several methodological improvements of the neuroimaging techniques and tasks used, as well as the assessment of creative answers, the time allowed for answering, etc.
- Operationalizing the difference between creative cognition and normative cognition. It should be further clarified how are these differences instantiated in the brain.
- Distinguishing between types of creativity. The authors suggested a division between creativity involved in problem solving (metaphor, analogy) and expression (art, verbal, music, design, architecture and dance).
Several recent meta-analyses have to some extent tried
to follow these suggested directions. Vartanian’s (2012) meta-analysis, for
instance, only included studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and
only those papers that focused on problem solving by analogy (10 papers) and
metaphor (10 papers). The analysis for analogy revealed two brain areas which
were reported in all studies to be activated – the dorsolateral and
rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. The analysis for metaphor revealed three activated brain
areas – the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, temporal pole and the
cingulate gyrus. The main conclusion was that the results of the meta-analysis do not support a unitary system or module for creativity in the brain. It was additionally
suggested that future meta-analyses should focus on specific cognitive
processes and be limited to a single neuroimaging modality.
Similar conclusions were put forward by Gonen-Yaacovi et al. (2013), who in their meta-analysis included 34 functional neuroimaging
studies based on the cardiovascular brain response (PET, fMRI and MRI). The
brain areas involved in creative cognition were predominantly located in the
left hemisphere, involving the caudal lateral prefrontal cortex, the medial and
lateral rostral prefrontal cortex, and the inferior parietal and posterior temporal
cortices. Combination tasks (e.g., Remote Associates Task) activated to a
greater extent anterior areas of the prefrontal cortex, whereas unusual
generation tasks (e.g., involving an explicit request to freely generate an
unusual response) activated caudal parts of the prefrontal cortex.
The recent meta-analysis by Pidgeon et al. (2016) was
directed to the analysis of visual creativity including 19 EEG and 7 fMRI
studies. The main findings were decreased alpha power reported in the EEG
studies and significant clusters in thalamus, left fusiform gyrus, and in the right
middle and inferior frontal gyrus observed in the fMRI studies. The authors
concluded that the results of the meta-analysis are consistent with suggested
contributions to visual creativity by means of prefrontal mediated inhibition,
evaluation, and working memory, as well as visual imagery processes.
A somewhat different approach was put forward by Heilman’s (2016) literature review. He
analyzed 3 stages involved in creative cognition – preparation, innovation and
creative production. The diversity of processes included in the meta-analysis
did not allow for a detailed picture of the brain-creativity relationship
and resembles the findings of prior
meta-analyses coming close to Sawyer’s (2011) conclusion: the entire brain
is active while individuals are engaged in creative cognition.
A more refined approach to study the brain-creativity
relation was suggested by Benedek et al. (2014). They tried to distinguish
between original answers to an alternate uses task, by asking subjects to classify
their answers whether they have retrieved them from long term memory or created them at that very moment. For example
two original answers to the request to generate creative uses for a car tire might be “using it for building a swing” or “using it as a picture frame”. The respondent
would then for instance indicate that the first answer was recalled, while the latter was a new idea. The main findings of the study were that new ideas occurred more frequently at
later stages in the ideation process. Divergent thinking was associated with
activation of the left ventrolateral and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices. The
generation of new ideas resulted in higher activation in the anterior part of
the left inferior parietal cortex including parts of the left supramarginal
gyrus as compared to retrieved answers.
This brief review demonstrates that recently there
have been some attempts to improve the design of correlational studies
investigating the brain-creativity relation. However, in a recent theoretical
review by Dietrich and Haider (2015, p. 897), it was concluded that “Despite a flurry of activity in cognitive
neuroscience, recent reviews have shown that there is no coherent picture
emerging from the neuroimaging work”. To overcome the shortcomings of
previous studies the authors proposed an evolutionary framework for the study
of the neurobiological underpinnings of creativity in which (partial) blind
variation and selective retention are central in a Darwinian adaptation process.
Creativity is a variational system that involves the partial coupling of
variation to selection subserved by internal representations of the emulated
future in that way providing foresight and purpose to human creativity. In
contrast, Gabora and Kauffman (2016, p. 638) objected the Darwinian approach,
suggesting that “shifting from a
Darwinian to a Lamarckian framework could be a step toward realizing their [Dietrich and Haider's]
vision of grounding creativity in an evolutionary prediction framework.”
Gabora and Kauffman (2016) vividly explained the
difference between what Dietrich and Haider suggested and their viewpoint of an
evolutionary approach to study creativity:
Let’s say you have three ideas A, B, and C that you
want to test for their effectiveness. You expose them (10 ideas of each type) to
selection criteria (evolution). In the
next generation there are 15 ideas of type A, 5 of type B, and 10 of type C. At
the end of the evolutionary process you have just variants of idea A. This
example demonstrates how Darwinian evolution works. However, we do not decide
for idea A because of the number of progenies it creates, but because we think
that A is the best choice. They suggested an alternative evolutionary model for
the study of creative cognition based on Lamarckian[1] evolutionary principles of
formal concept combination.
From a pragmatic viewpoint the main obstacle for
substantial research in the domain is the unsolved issue of how creative
cognition should be assessed. At our lab we were among the first who investigated
neurobiological underpinnings of creativity (Jaušovec, 1996). Later we abandoned
this research strand mainly because of the lack of valid and reliable tests of
creative cognition.
References
Abraham, A. (2013). The promises and perils of the
neuroscience of creativity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00246
Arden, R., Chavez, R. S., Grazioplene, R., & Jung,
R. E. (2010). Neuroimaging creativity: A psychometric view. Behavioural Brain
Research, 214(2), 143–156. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.015
Benedek, M., Jauk,
E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., & Neubauer, A. C.
(2014). To
create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative
new ideas. NeuroImage, 88, 125–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.021
Deary, I. J. (2000). Looking down on human
intelligence: from psychometrics to the brain. Oxford ; New York: Oxford
University Press.
Dietrich, A., & Haider, H. (2015). Human
creativity, evolutionary algorithms, and predictive representations: The
mechanics of thought trials. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(4), 897–915.
https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-014-0743-x
Dietrich, A., & Kanso, R. (2010). A review of EEG,
ERP, and neuroimaging studies of creativity and insight. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 822–848.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019749
Gabora, L., &
Kauffman, S. (2016). Toward an evolutionary-predictive foundation
for creativity: Commentary on “Human creativity, evolutionary algorithms, and
predictive representations: The mechanics of thought trials” by Arne Dietrich
and Hilde Haider, 2014 (Accepted pending minor revisions for publication in
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review,
23(2), 632–639. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0925-1
Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American
Psychologist, 5, 444–454. doi:10.1037/h0063487
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human
intelligence. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Gonen-Yaacovi, G., de Souza, L. C., Levy, R.,
Urbanski, M., Josse, G., & Volle, E. (2013). Rostral and caudal prefrontal
contribution to creativity: a meta-analysis of functional imaging data.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00465
Heilman, K. M. (2016). Possible Brain Mechanisms of
Creativity. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 31(4), 285–296.
https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acw009
Jaušovec, N. (1996). Differences in EEG alpha activity
related to giftedness. Intelligence, 23(3), 159–173.
https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(96)90001-X
Pidgeon, L. M., Grealy, M., Duffy, A. H. B., Hay, L.,
McTeague, C., Vuletic, T., … Gilbert, S. J. (2016). Functional neuroimaging of
visual creativity: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain and Behavior,
6(10), e00540. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.540
Sawyer, K. (2011). The Cognitive Neuroscience of
Creativity: A Critical Review. Creativity Research Journal, 23(2), 137–154.
https://doi.org/10.1080/10400419.2011.571191
Vartanian, O. (2012). Dissociable neural systems for
analogy and metaphor: Implications for the neuroscience of creativity: Brain
and creativity. British Journal of Psychology, 103(3), 302–316.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.2011.02073.x
[1] Lamarckism
is understood to refer to an evolutionary process in which traits acquired
during an organism’s lifetime can be transmitted to offspring without the
mechanism of genetics.
I am contemplating a relatively brief theoretico-methodological article about (a) the relationship of "creativity" to the brain and (b) prerequisites and constraints on creativity. I find this blog useful and would like, possibly, to cite it. the only potential author ID appears to be: "Posted by Norbert at 7:57 AM apparently on January 30 2017. This is my first exposure to this website and blog. Would Norbert please identify her/himself by emailing me at rkthomas@uga.edu?
ReplyDeleteCreativity is about the right brain. If you can think more creatively then you might be a right brain thinker and also you could perform well at music, academics.
ReplyDeleteMemory Improvement Techniquest
Concentration Exercises For Students
Kids Brain Trainer
Right Brain Training
Physics And Maths Tutor
One On One Maths Tutoring
Free from Herpes just in 2 weeks
ReplyDeleteThe best herbal remedy
You can contact him
r.buckler11 {{@gmail}} com,, .......