Cognitive training can be roughly divided into strategy and
core, or domain-general training. Von Bastian and Oberauer (2013) based their
classification of training approaches on transfer mechanisms, enhancing either memory efficiency or memory
capacity. Strategy training is usually defined as targeting task-specific
strategies that might help trainees to improve memory performance, most often
resembling different mnemonic techniques. This transfer yields to increased
memory efficiency providing the possibility for acquiring knowledge and skills
such as strategy usage, chunk learning, and automation of basic processes and
in that way decreasing the time needed to move the focus of attention between
single items.
Theory
Both approaches, strategy as well as core training, target working memory. The difference between them is in the specific
mechanisms they assume intelligence
and working memory
share. This aspect to a great extent shapes the training tasks that they advocate.
We discussed core training in detail in our previous
blog about the n-back,
as well as in our forthcoming book Increasing Intelligence,
therefore only a brief recap will be presented here. Core training involves the repetition of
demanding working memory (WM) tasks that target domain-general WM mechanisms assumed
to overlap with the same cognitive resources needed in complex problem solving.
Furthermore, it was suggested that repetitive cognitive demands that exceed
resource limits also induce changes in brain regions related to working memory
performance (Taya et al., 2015). More simplified, working memory and
intelligence share a common capacity constraint, thus by increasing WM
capacity, intelligence should also increase (Buschkuehl and Jaeggi, 2010). Although it is usually not explicitly stated that the assumed intelligence – WM shared component is
capacity. This is in line with Cowan’s (Cowan et al. 2005) and Colom’s (Colom et
al., 2008) research findings that short term storage largely accounts for the
relation between intelligence and working memory.
While the emphasize of core training is to minimize the
possibility for the development of task specific strategies, in contrast, the goal of strategic training is to develop specific strategies that would
allow better working memory performance.
Engle (Engle et al., 1999) suggested that executive
functioning, especially control of
attention over interference and conflict, is the central process contributing to
the shared variance with intelligence. Their idea has been further developed
suggesting that when subjects are confronted with a complex working memory test
where they have to solve mathematical equations between the presentation of the
to-be-remembered items, they have to displace the items from primary to
secondary memory. The same happens for long lists in a memory span task (more
than 4 items). The relation with intelligence derives from these two processes,
the ability to retain information in primary memory and to recover it from
secondary memory (Unsworth and Engle, 2007).
Even more extreme is the position that only the ability to retrieve information from secondary memory
contributes to the shared variance between working memory and intelligence
(Mogle et al., 2008). However, this finding could not be replicated (Shelton et al., 2010). Yet another process identified as crucial for the intelligence-WM overlap was relational integration, defined as the ability of building new
relations between elements and thereby creating structural representations
(Oberauer et al., 2008).
Thus it is not surprising that in a recent paper Engle suggested that because core programs showed no lasting far transfer effects, we should
turn to lower-cost learning-strategy training programs. They concluded: “Working-memory training as currently
implemented does not work. One hundred years of research on basic memory
phenomena has discovered many procedures that do!” (McCabe et al., 2016,
p. 190).
Types of strategy training
In a more recent review by Dunlasky et al. (2013), ten strategies
were proposed:
- Elaborative interrogation: Generating an explanation why an explicitly stated fact or concept is true. In other words, the strategy requests the trainee to ask: Why? This prompts the learners to generate explanations. The strategy was rated as having a moderate utility.
- Self-explanation: Explaining how new information is related to known information, or explaining steps taken during problem solving. The strategy was rated as having a moderate utility.
- Summarization: Students are prompted to write summaries of the to-be-learned text thereby identifying the main points while excluding unimportant material. The strategy was rated as having a low utility.
- Highlighting/underlining is probably one of the most often used strategies by students. Marked or underlined are potentially important portions of text while reading. The strategy does not tend to show beneficial effects on learning and was rated as having a low utility.
- Keyword mnemonic is a technique developed for learning foreign language vocabulary. The student is prompted to relate the word to-be learned in foreign language with one in mother tongue that sounds similar. For instance the French word “la dent” (tooth) with the English “dentist”. The strategy was rated as having a low utility.
- Imagery is one of the most researched strategies. Students are asked to mentally imagine the to-be learned word or content using simple and clear mental images. It is assumed that in utilizing multiple modalities for encoding, the number of routes for retrieval are enhanced (e.g., Paivio's 1986 dual-coding theory). The strategy was rated as having a low utility.
- Rereading is yet another technique frequently used by students during self-regulated learning. The strategy was rated as having a low utility.
- Practice testing can involve several forms such as recall of target information with flashcards, completing practice problems given at the end of chapters, or completing practice tests included in the electronic materials or via E-learning platforms. The strategy was rated as having a high utility.
- Distributed practice means that studying is distributed over time (either within a single study session or across sessions), which is opposite to the most often used strategy by students, namely to study only immediately prior to a test and falsely believing that this cramming strategy is effective. From a theoretical viewpoint the cramming strategy may actually interfere with the learned material providing the student with a false impression of knowing the newly learned material. Another aspect coming from neuroscience is memory consolidation. Distributed practice was rated as having a high utility.
- Interleaved practice is the opposite of blocking study, in which all of the content from one subtopic is studied before the student moves on to the next set of materials. It is a holistic approach in which students alternate their practice with different kinds of problems. The strategy was rated as having a moderate utility.
In addition to the 10 strategies proposed by Dunlasky et
al. (2013), Ritchey and Nokes-Malach (2015) suggested
the strategy of analogical comparison for science learning. The student is exposed to multiple examples that share the same general
principles but have different superficial features. The extracted principle is
then applied to novel problems. The strategy is assumed to enhance the
relations within and between problem domains, thus it should influence also far
transfer effects. However, the main problem is that students seldom find the common
principle when surface characteristics differ, even when provided with a hint.
Another group of strategies can be categorized as mnemonics,
which are either verbal or visual or the combination of both. The common
underlying principle of mnemonic techniques is to impose a chunking scheme on
material and by that making the material meaningful to the learner (McCabe et
al., 2016). The most often used mnemonic techniques are firstletter (e.g.,
acronyms, acrostics), keywords (see point 5. of the list provided by Dunlasky et al., 2013), the pegword method, the method of loci, and the use of songs,
rhymes, and stories. In addition, Gross et al. (2012) in their meta-analysis
included:
- Categorization which involves recognizing and grouping items by conceptual relations.
- Associations - linking items with respect to time, environment, or specific characteristics, combining them with images, senses, words, and sentences.
- The method of loci is a visuo-spatial mnemonic technique that pairs location (known places, such as body parts, or landmarks on the way to work) with to-be-remembered items.
- External memory aids are environmental cues such as lists, reminder notes, calendars, and messages to one’s self.
Near and far transfer effects
Compared to the number of studies on core training effects, there
is much less recent research dealing with strategy training. The beneficial effect
of strategic training has been mainly investigated in older populations (Gross
et al., 2012; Lustig et al., 2009; Rebok et al., 2007; Verhaeghen et al., 1992).
In a review paper by Verhaeghen et al. (1992), effect sizes for mnemonics such
as loci (d=.80), name-face (d=.83), peg-word (d=.62), imagery (d=.14), and
organization (d=.85) were reported. Rebok et al. (2007) stressed that multiple
mnemonic approaches in comparison to single ones lead to more successful
transfer to everyday memory tasks. Lustig et al. (2009, p. 505) analyzing 11
mnemonic strategy training studies concluded: “…these studies often found large and long-lasting effects on the
trained task, but only limited evidence for transfer.”
In a more recent meta-analysis by Gross et al. (2012), in which 35 studies were reviewed, an overall effect size of 0.31 was reported. It was further shown
that training gains were not related to strategy type, average age of
participants, session length, or type of control condition. With respect to
far/near transfer effects the meta-analysis did not provide any information, because
the different measures of pre- posttest were collapsed into a single one of
memory performance. However, in explaining the rather low effect sizes obtained, the authors pointed out that probably the main reason was the heterogeneity of
memory tasks used in pre- posttests in the studies.
In a recent study by Li et al. (2016), pure memory strategy
training (method of loci and face-name mnemonic) was compared with a combined cognitive training approach that consisted of executive function training (updating and switching
training) and memory strategy training. The results showed that pure memory strategy training significantly improved (in comparison to a no-contact control group) performance on memory
tasks (mainly trained ones, but also on a composite score including untrained
memory tasks). In this group, no effects were observed on executive function tasks.
The study by Carretti et al. (2007) was the only one that
reported a far transfer effect of strategy training on working memory. The
authors used imagery training prompting the trainees to create an image for each word and to
interrelate the images they constructed. The
pre- posttest tasks were an immediate list recall task asking subjects to recall
15 words presented at a rate of 2s per word (near transfer task). The second
task was a categorization working memory
span task, similar to complex memory span tasks such as the reading span task.
Instead of sentences, lists of words (3-6 in each series) were read, asking
subjects to tap their hand on the table whenever they heard an animal noun and
to remember the last word in each series. At the end of the series, the participants had to recall the last word of each list in serial order (far
transfer task). The authors concluded: “The
results of the current paper strongly confirmed the positive effect of
strategic training for enhancing the performance in a working memory task and
in immediate recall both for the younger and older experimental group”
(Carretti et al., 2007, p. 317). In our opinion the conclusion of a far transfer
does not seem justified because the training and the criterion task (categorization
working memory span task) were rather similar. It is interesting that in a
follow up study the same authors used a core training approach. In this study the
training involved just the categorization working memory span task used in
their first study as the far transfer task (Borella et al., 2010). The core training
showed near and far transfer effects on WM and intelligence.
This short review has demonstrated that there is little
evidence that strategy or mnemonic training could influence intelligence. The
only exception is analogical comparison strategy training. However, as pointed
out by Ritchey and Nokes-Malach (2015), students seldom succeed to identify the
analogy when surface problem characteristics differ.
In one of our earlier
studies we tried to analyze the relationship between analogical reasoning and intelligence
(Jaušovec, 1993). For that purpose 4 experiments were conducted. In the first
experiment, highly intelligent and average intelligent undergraduates were presented with a
story analogy before an attempt to solve the insight radiation problem. Intelligent
individuals did not produce significantly more dispersion solutions (enhanced
by the analogy) than average ones. In experiment 2, the same students as in the
first experiment solved 6 problems while thinking aloud. Intelligent
individuals used significantly more analogies (most often association
analogies, but sometimes transformation analogies) than the average intelligent
group of students. In the third experiment, students were trained in strategies
used in problem solving (e.g., means end, analogical reasoning). They solved 3
problems before and after training, while thinking aloud. Training affected problem
solving performance but not analogical reasoning. In the last experiment
students were given training geared more toward improving analogical reasoning
abilities. Effects of this training were less impressive than effects of
training in other strategies.
In conclusion at the moment it does not seem that strategy
training is the right choice in the endeavor
to increase intelligence or to further elucidate its
understanding.
References
Borella, E., Carretti, B., Riboldi, F., & De Beni, R.
(2010). Working memory training in older adults: Evidence of transfer and
maintenance effects. Psychology and Aging, 25(4), 767–778.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020683
Buschkuehl,
M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2010). Improving intelligence: A literature review.
Swiss Medical Weekly, 140(19-20), 266–272.
Carretti, B., Borella, E., & De Beni, R. (2007). Does
Strategic Memory Training Improve the Working Memory Performance of Younger and
Older Adults? Experimental Psychology, 54(4), 311–320.
https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169.54.4.311
Colom, R., Abad, F. J., Quiroga, M. Á., Shih, P. C., &
Flores-Mendoza, C. (2008). Working memory and intelligence are highly related
constructs, but why? Intelligence, 36(6), 584–606. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.002
Cowan, N., Elliott, E. M., Scott Saults, J., Morey, C. C.,
Mattox, S., Hismjatullina, A., & Conway, A. R. A. (2005). On the capacity
of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive
aptitudes. Cognitive Psychology, 51(1), 42–100. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.12.001
Dunlosky, J., Rawson, K. A., Marsh, E. J., Nathan, M. J.,
& Willingham, D. T. (2013). Improving Students’ Learning With Effective
Learning Techniques: Promising Directions From Cognitive and Educational
Psychology. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 14(1), 4–58. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100612453266
Engle, R.W., Tuholski, S.W., Laughlin, J.E., & Conway,
A.R.A. (1999). Working memory, shortterm memory and general fluid intelligence:
A latent variable approach. Journal of ExperimentalPsychology:General,128,
309–331.
Gross, A. L., Parisi, J. M., Spira, A. P., Kueider, A. M.,
Ko, J. Y., Saczynski, J. S., … Rebok, G. W. (2012). Memory training
interventions for older adults: A meta-analysis. Aging & Mental Health,
16(6), 722–734. https://doi.org/10.1080/13607863.2012.667783
Jaušovec, N.
(1993). The influence of ability on analogical transfer. Ricerche di
Psicologia, Vol 17(2), 37-50.
Li, B., Zhu, X., Hou, J., Chen, T., Wang, P., & Li, J.
(2016). Combined Cognitive Training vs. Memory Strategy Training in Healthy
Older Adults. Frontiers in
Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00834
Lustig, C., Shah,
P., Seidler, R., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2009). Aging, Training, and
the Brain: A Review and Future Directions. Neuropsychology Review, 19(4),
504–522. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-009-9119-9
McCabe, J. A., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2016).
Brain-Training Pessimism, but Applied-Memory Optimism. Psychological Science in
the Public Interest, 17(3), 187–191. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100616664716
Mogle, J. A.,
Lovett, B. J., Stawski, R. S., & Sliwinski, M. J. (2008). What’s so special
about working memory? An examination of the relationships among working memory,
secondary memory, and fluid intelligence. Psychological
Science, 19, 1071–1077.
Oberauer, K., Süβ, H.-M., Wilhelm, O., & Wittmann, W.
W. (2008). Which working memory functions predict intelligence?
Intelligence, 36(6), 641–652. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.007
Paivio, A.
(1986). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford, England: Oxford
University Press.
Rebok, G. W.,
Carlson, M. C., & Langbaurn, J. B. S. (2007). Training and maintaining
memory abilities in healthy older adults: traditional and novel approaches.
Journals of Gerontology Series B-Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences,
62, 53–61.
Richey, J. E., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2015). Comparing
Four Instructional Techniques for Promoting Robust Knowledge. Educational
Psychology Review, 27(1), 181–218. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-014-9268-0
Shelton, J.
T., Elliott, E. M., Matthews, R. A., Hill, B. D., & Gouvier, W. D. (2010).
The relationships of working memory, secondary memory, and general fluid
intelligence: Working memory is special. Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(3), 813–820. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0019046
Taya, F.,
Sun, Y., Babiloni, F., Thakor, N., & Bezerianos, A. (2015). Brain
enhancement through cognitive training: a new insight from brain connectome.
Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 9. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00044
Unsworth, N.,
and Engle, R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory
capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from
secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114, 104−132.
Verhaeghen P, Marcoen A, Goossens L. Improving memory
performance in the aged through mnemonic training: A meta-analytic study.
Psychology and Aging. 1992; 7:242–251. [PubMed: 1535198]
von Bastian, C. C., & Oberauer, K. (2013). Effects and
mechanisms of working memory training: a review. Psychological Research, 78(6),
803–820. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0524-6
KADAN: Online casino in the state of TANU - KDAR
ReplyDeleteKADAN: Online casino in the 카지노 state of TANU - KDAR KADAN. 1.9 km from Mainz and the centre of TANU. kadangpintar The nearest casino to KAKAN is worrione Jupitersk and Kurang
Free from Herpes just in 2 weeks
ReplyDeleteThe best herbal remedy
You can contact him
r.buckler11 {{@gmail}} com,, .......