Several anecdotal descriptions of big and small inventions mention
the “aha” experience, a sudden unpredictable discovery following prolonged unsuccessful
solution attempts. The Nobel laureate and occasional Feynman (also a Nobel laureate)
collaborator, Murray Gell-Mann (leaning
against the blackboard) described the magic solution process of Feynman to his
student: “Dick’s method is this. You write
down the problem. You think very hard. (Gell-Mann shuts down his eyes and
presses his knuckles periodically to his forehead.) Then you write down the
answer.” In Seyfert’s (Seyfert et al., 1994, p. 72-73) opinion this
description is an example of the “wizard Merlin” perspective on insight.
Beside the unexpected appearance of the solution, anecdotal reports
of insight have also revealed that the answers were most often experienced in a
dream[1],
or in a state of day-dreaming and mind wondering characterized by diminished consciousness.
The discovery of the benzene ring by Kekulé represents such an example
(Seyfert et al., 1994, p. 115):
“I turned my chair to
the fire and doze… Again the atoms gamboling before my eyes… My mental eye…
could now distinguish larger structures, of manifold conformation; long rows,
sometimes more closely fitted together; all twining and twisting in a snakelike
motion. But look! … One of the snakes had seized hold of its own tail, and the
form whirled mockingly before my eyes. As if by a flash of lightening I awoke.”
Yet another characteristic described was the immense
emotional intensity of insight moments. For instance, in the almost archetypal
story of the aha-moment, Archimedes of
Syracuse was so inspired by his discovery that he supposedly ran down the
street shouting ‘‘Eureka!’’ without remembering to put on his clothes.
Some reports point also to a coincidental event that triggered
a mapping process with a previously unsolved problem – such as the famous
Newton’s apple. As the legend goes, Newton went for a walk in the countryside.
It was autumn and he happened to notice an apple fall from a tree. On seeing
this, it suddenly occurred to him that the moon is like an apple towards Earth,
which let him to formulate his gravitational law.
From a historical perspective, the first who were attracted
by the insight phenomenon were Gestaltists. Graham Wallas (1926), for example, proposed four stages involved in
the creative solution process:
preparation, incubation, illumination or insight, and verification. Especially the processes of
incubation and illumination were in the focus
of research interest. As stressed by Davidson and Sternberg (2003), the gestalt
analysis of insight was later followed
by approaches that did not recognize that insight problems are something
special and those who tried to study insight in the lab by employing different puzzle problems. Recent research shows a
preference for neurobiological and genetic explanations of the insight experience. Davidson and Sternberg (2003) further
concluded that insightful problem solving differs from working out solutions of
interpolation and logic problems. Insight involves searching for overlooked
relevant encodings, combinations, and comparisons of information and the
restructuring of one’s mental representation of the problem. Highly intelligent
individuals are better at this process than individuals of average
intelligence.
For the incubation phase preceding illumination, several
functions have been suggested. Selective forgetting, which eliminates false leads and hampering assumptions, is thought to occur during this process. Recent review papers suggested that during the incubation period unconscious processes
contribute to creative thinking (e.g., Gilhooly, 2016; Ritter and Dijksterhuis,
2014). It was further suggested that in this unconscious thinking process a
joint effect of spreading activation (unusual associations) and subconscious
goal activation (rising train of associations) subserves creative solutions.
Sio and Ormerod (2009) included
117 (n = 3606) independent studies in their meta-analysis of incubation. The unweighted mean of
the unbiased effect size estimate was 0.41, suggesting moderate incubation
effects. The main subconscious processes identified were activation of new
knowledge, or restructuring, and forgetting carried out either by switching the
strategy used, or by relaxing inappropriate self-imposed constraints on the
problem representation. For creative problem solving, longer incubation periods
were more beneficial than for linguistic
problems, for which a low cognitive load
task during the incubation gave the strongest effects.
Neurocognitive explanations
Kuonious and Beeman (2014) in a recent review on the
neurocognitive underpinnings of incubation and insight identified several
characteristics of brain activity that can be nicely integrated into a neuro
cognitive theory of insight (selective forgetting, or restructuring of
problems). The main conclusion put forward was that coarse semantic coding in
the right hemisphere subserves the insight process. It was further suggested
that in association cortices of Wernicke’s and Broca’s areas, and the anterior
temporal cortex, right hemisphere neurons have larger input fields than left
hemisphere neurons do, which physiologically enhances wider spreading of activity.
EEG studies in the right hemisphere revealed bursts of gamma and alpha band
activity, a pattern that suggests intense mental work (gamma-band) accompanied
by suppression of distracting environmental stimuli (alpha band activity in the
right occipital lobe). This switching process between inwardly and outwardly
directed attention was accompanied with high/low anterior cingulate activity. Inwardly
directed attention and high anterior cingulate activity heightens sensitivity
to weakly activated remote associations and long-shot solution ideas and vice versa.
Individuals also differed in their tendency to solve verbal anagram puzzles
either analytically or by insight. Furthermore their thinking style was related
to the level of arousal prior to task
presentation (2 s). Insightful individuals showed greater right hemisphere
activity at rest, relative to analytic individuals.
The authors concluded (Kuonious and Beeman, 2014; p.83): “Thus, the notion that insight is associated
with diffuse attention appears to be an oversimplification. Insightful
individuals may generally have more diffuse and outwardly directed attention,
but successful insight solving involves transiently redirecting attention
inwardly during the preparation for and solving of a problem. It therefore
appears that the tendency to solve problems insightfully is associated with
broad perceptual intake as the default mode of resting-state attention
deployment, coupled with the tendency to focus inwardly in preparation for, and
during, solving. In contrast, analytical people’s resting-state attention is
less outwardly focused during the resting state and less inwardly directed
during preparation and solving.”
A similar explanation was also provided for the beneficial influence of non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep on incubation, insight and creativity observed in several studies
(e.g., Drago et al., 2011; Landmann et al., 2014; Llewellyn, 2016). It was
proposed that sleep promotes the qualitative reorganization of memories. Low
levels of cortical arousal during NREM sleep may enhance the ability to access
remote associations that are critical for creative innovations. Cyclic Alternating Patterns (CAP) during NREM also play a role in this process. They are characterized by
periodic transient events (phase A of CAP) arising from background activity
(phase B). The A phase is subdivided into A1-3 subtypes based on the relative
proportions of slow wave and fast EEG oscillations. The A1 subtype (most common) is characterized by a
prevalence of high-voltage slow waves in the frontal brain areas, which have been identified to be important
for divergent thinking and creativity. These
explanations are also in line with
findings suggesting the importance of slow-wave sleep on other forms of
cognitive functions, such as learning and processing speed (Ferri et al.,
2008). The main mechanisms of these effects are explained by the synaptic
homeostasis hypothesis and the spreading activation theory which complement
each other. The former proposes a global
downscaling of synaptic strength during slow-wave sleep that improves the
signal-to-noise ratio, whereas the latter suggest that similar memory traces are
linked together to form a novel network subserved by occurring changes in synaptic connectivity
in localized neuronal circuits (Landmann et al., 2014).
Recent advances in molecular genetics have made it possible
to analyze the underlying genetic architectures of individual differences in
the tendency to solve problems analytically or via insight. Zhang and Zhang
(2016) explored the association of dopamine D2 receptor gene (DRD2) with
insight problem solving. The study provided evidence for a positive association
between genotyped SNPs, rs1800497 and rs6278 with spatial insight problem
solving. Because for verbal insight problems this relation was not so prominent
it was suggested that the findings lend support for the domain-specific theory
of insight, rather than a unitary category of creative problem solving.
It seems that neuroimaging and genetic research step by step
uncover the mystery of the “aha moment” so often mentioned in anecdotal reports
of great discoveries and inventions.
References
Davidson, J. E.,
& Sternberg, R. J. (2003). The psychology of problem solving. Cambridge,
UK; New York: Cambridge University Press. Retrieved from
http://www.books24x7.com/marc.asp?bookid=8963
Drago, V.,
Foster, P. S., Heilman, K. M., Aricò, D., Williamson, J., Montagna, P., &
Ferri, R. (2011). Cyclic alternating pattern in sleep and its relationship to
creativity. Sleep Medicine, 12(4), 361–366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleep.2010.11.009
Ferri R, Huber R,
Aricò D, et al. The slow-wave components of the cyclic alternating pattern
(CAP) have a role in sleep-related learning processes. Neurosci Lett 2008; 432:
228–31.
Gilhooly, K. J.
(2016). Incubation and Intuition in Creative Problem Solving. Frontiers in
Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01076
Haider, H., &
Rose, M. (2007). How to investigate insight: A proposal. Methods, 42(1), 49–57.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ymeth.2006.12.004
Kounios, J.,
& Beeman, M. (2014). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Insight. Annual Review
of Psychology, 65(1), 71–93.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115154
Landmann, N.,
Kuhn, M., Piosczyk, H., Feige, B., Baglioni, C., Spiegelhalder, K., … Nissen,
C. (2014). The reorganisation of memory during sleep. Sleep Medicine Reviews,
18(6), 531–541. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2014.03.005
Llewellyn, S.
(2016). Crossing the invisible line: De-differentiation of wake, sleep and
dreaming may engender both creative insight and psychopathology. Consciousness
and Cognition, 46, 127–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.09.018
Ritter, S. M.,
& Dijksterhuis, A. (2014). Creativity: the unconscious foundations of the
incubation period. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00215
Seifert, C. M.,
Meyer, D. E., Davidson, N., Patalano, A. L., & Yaniv, I. (1994).
Demystification of cognitive insight: Opportunistic assimilation and the
prepared-mind hypothesis. In R. Sternberg, & J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature
of insight (pp. 65-124). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Sio, U. N., &
Ormerod, T. C. (2009). Does incubation enhance problem solving? A meta-analytic
review. Psychological Bulletin, 135(1), 94–120. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014212
Wallas, G.
(1926). The art of thought. New York: Harcourt, Brace.
Zhang, S., &
Zhang, J. (2016). The Association of DRD2 with Insight Problem Solving.
Frontiers in Psychology, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01865
[1] Beatle
Paul McCartney announced that he came up with the melody for “Yesterday” in a
dream.
Free from Herpes just in 2 weeks
ReplyDeleteThe best herbal remedy
You can contact him
r.buckler11 {{@gmail}} com,, .......