Intelligence represents the individual’s overall level of
intellectual ability. It serves as a general concept that includes several
groups of mental abilities. Historically speaking, one of the first divisions
of intelligence splits it into verbal, performance and social intelligence[1]
(Thorndike, 1920). Social intelligence has been described as the ability to
understand and manage people, and to act wisely in social situations (Thorndike
& Stein, 1937). More recently, Cantor and Kihlstrom (1985, 1987) proposed
social intelligence as a construct referring to a central personality process
that underpins social behavior.
Over the last two decades, the concept of emotional
intelligence (EI) – which can be viewed as the latest incarnation of the social
intelligence concept – has received much
attention in the popular and scientific literature. Different definitions and operationalizations of EI exist, which can be roughly divided into (1)
ability models and (2) mixed (traits
with abilities) models (Joseph and Newman, 2010). Besides ability models, some authors identified several additional subcategories (e.g., Tsaousis and Kazi, 2013). The
first is Bar-On’s model of emotional and social intelligence. In Bar-On's
(2000) opinion, EI is a composite of different facets compromising cognitive,
motivational and affective constructs
including five components: intrapersonal
skills, interpersonal skills, adaptability, stress management, and general
mood. The second model encompasses the social and emotional competencies:
self-awareness, self-management, social awareness, and relationship management
(Boyatzis and Sala, 2004). The third most recent model is the trait emotional
intelligence model by Petrides et al. (2007) measuring typical performance of
four components: well-being, sociability, self-control, and emotionality. Some
authors have further suggested that it is the type of measurement rather than
the theory per se that determines the nature of the model (e.g., Petrides et
al., 2007). Joseph and Newman (2010) for instance suggested the division into:
performance-based ability EI, self-report ability EI, and self-report mixed EI.
On the other hand, the most influential ability or information-processing model is the one by Mayer et al. (2000; 2002; 2008 a,b). They define EI as the ability to recognize emotion, reason with emotion and emotion-related information, and process emotional information as part of general problem-solving (see Figure 1).
On the other hand, the most influential ability or information-processing model is the one by Mayer et al. (2000; 2002; 2008 a,b). They define EI as the ability to recognize emotion, reason with emotion and emotion-related information, and process emotional information as part of general problem-solving (see Figure 1).
Figure 1 The main
components of Emotional Intelligence
To measure these branches empirically, the authors developed
the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT). The test consists of performance tasks requiring responses that are evaluated
against predetermined scoring criteria.
The central, still unsolved, questions about the ability EI
construct are: first, do these four branches link up to a higher-order global EI
factor supporting the idea of a unified area of ability – emotional
intelligence – that can be subdivided into subsidiary areas of skill, and
second, does the MSCEIT measure
something that differs from IQ or personality?
A concern raised by several researchers was that one could
question the usefulness of EI for enhancing the understanding of the
determinants of human performance (e.g., Schulte et al., 2004; De Raad, 2005). This
concern was supported by empirical evidence. Schulte and colleagues (2004), for
instance, reported a corrected multiple R = 0.81 between EI and other well-known constructs: the big five personality dimensions and g. Similar
findings were reported by De Raad (2005) and Fiori and Antonakis (2011). In the
study by de Rad, 42% of emotional
intelligence items were accommodated by the big five personality factor of emotional stability. The study by Fiori and Antonakis found that the MSCEIT is
largely predicted by personality dimensions, general intelligence, and
demographics having multiple R’s with the MSCEIT branches up to 0.66; for the
general EI factor this relation was even stronger (Multiple R = 0.76).
Furthermore, the low intercorrelations among the EI branches posed some doubts about
the presence of an underlying higher-order EI factor. The authors suggested
that the problems could be approached by revising the scoring process based on
consensus/expert scores. In consensus scoring the highest scores are obtained for
answers corresponding to those given by the majority of individuals in a large
predetermined database. The evidence for the validity of this type of scoring
is the high correlation between consensus and expert scores, which raises the question of the
discriminatory characteristics of the test. For example, it would be rather
unusual if gifted individuals on an IQ test provided the same responses as the majority of respondents. This would suggest that the test cannot
sort high from average IQ individuals.
Two recent studies adopted a different theoretical perspective to tackle
the question whether EI can be distinguished from IQ. Gutiérrez-Cobo et al. (2016) instead of correlating
EI branches with IQ test scores, performed an analysis on problem solving tasks.
They divided the latter into “cool” (i.e., not emotionally laden) and “hot”
(i.e., emotionally laden) laboratory tasks. An example of a “hot” task is the IGT, where
participants have to select one hundred different cards from four decks. The
four decks contain unequal monetary punishments and rewards; the goal is to
obtain as much money as possible. Examples of “cool” tasks were digit span and
the symbol inspection time task, in which subjects were asked to discriminate between two symbols: + and x. The analysis for “hot” cognitive tasks showed
that higher-EI individuals tended to perform better in these cognitive tasks, whereas
for the “cool” tasks no relation was observed, which would to some extent
support the distinctiveness of the EI construct, at least in relation to IQ.
The central question put forward by Pietsching and Gittler
(2017) was whether EI exhibits potential test score changes over time, similar
to the dynamic generational changes of standardized IQ psychometric test
performance in the general population known as the Flynn effect (Flynn, 1987).
The meta-analysis included data from 160 samples (N = 16,738) from
English-speaking countries over a time-span of 15 years (2001–2015). The main
finding was that no substantial generational test score changes in overall or
branch EI scores were observed. The authors suggested that one of the possible reasons
for such a pattern may well be “that
ability emotional intelligence is indeed a distinct intelligence domain that
remains genuinely unaffected by causes that acted as boosters for other
intelligence domains (e.g., fluid, crystallized, or spatial IQ)”
(Pietsching and Gittler, 2017, p. 7).
Despite these shortcomings with respect to the concept and measurement
of emotional intelligence, several recent studies have investigated the
relationship between EI and aggression/deviant behavior, leadership, health/well-being and brain activity - topics
that we will address in our forthcoming blogs.
References
Bar-On, R. (2000). Emotional
and social intelligence: Insights from the Emotional Quotient Inventory. In R.
Bar-On, & J. D. A. Parker (Eds.), The handbook of emotional intelligence
(pp. 363–388). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
Boyatzis, R. E., & Sala,
F. (2004). Assessing emotional intelligence competencies. In G. Geher (Ed.),
Measuring emotional intelligence. Common ground and controversy (pp. 147–180).
Hauppage, NY: Nova Science.
Cantor, N., & Kihlstrom,
J. F. (1985). Social intelligence: The cognitive basis of personality. In P.
Shaver (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 15–33).
Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
Cantor, N., & Kihlstrom,
J. F. (1987). Personality and social intelligence. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
De Raad, B. (2005). The
trait-coverage of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual
Differences, 38, 673-687.
Fiori, M., &
Antonakis, J. (2011). The ability model of emotional intelligence: Searching
for valid measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(3), 329–334.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.10.010
Flynn, J. R. (1987). Massive
IQ gains in 14 nations: What IQ tests really measure. Psychological Bulletin,
101, 171–191.
Gutiérrez-Cobo, M. J.,
Cabello, R., & Fernández-Berrocal, P. (2016). The Relationship between
Emotional Intelligence and Cool and Hot Cognitive Processes: A Systematic
Review. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10.
https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00101
Joseph, D. L., & Newman,
D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: An integrative meta-analysis and
cascading model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 54–78.
https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017286
Mayer, J. D., Roberts, R. D.,
& Barsade, S. G. (2008a). Human Abilities: Emotional Intelligence. Annual
Review of Psychology, 59(1), 507–536.
https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093646
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P.,
& Caruso, D. R. (2008b). Emotional intelligence: New ability or eclectic
traits? American Psychologist, 63(6), 503–517. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.63.6.503
Mayer, J.D., Caruso, D.R. and
Salovey, P. (2000). Emotional intelligence meets traditional standards for an
intelligence. Intelligence, 27, 267-298.
Mayer, J.D., Salovey, P.,
& Caruso, D.R. (2002).
Mayer-Salovey-Caruso emotional intelligence test (MSCEIT). Toronto: MHS.
Petrides, K. V., Pita, R.,
& Kokkinaki, F. (2007). The location of trait emotional intelligence in
personality factor space. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 273–289.
Pietschnig, J., &
Gittler, G. (2017). Is ability-based emotional intelligence impervious to the
Flynn effect? A cross-temporal meta-analysis (2001–2015). Intelligence.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2016.12.006
Schulte, M.J., Ree, M.J.,
& Carretta, T.R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: not much more than g and
personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 37, 1059-1068.
Thorndike, E.,L.
(1920). Intelligence and its use. Harper’s Magazine, 140, 227-235.
Thorndike, R. L.,
& Stein, S. (1937). An evaluation of the attempts to measure social
intelligence. Psychological Bulletin, 34, 275–285.
Tsaousis, I.,
& Kazi, S. (2013). Factorial invariance and latent mean differences of
scores on trait emotional intelligence across gender and age. Personality and
Individual Differences, 54(2), 169–173.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.08.016
[1] Some authors have argued that the concept was
first introduced by Dewey in 1909 (Joseph and Newman, 2010)
More about the author gucci dolabuy visite site Louis Vuitton replica Bags navigate here hermes dolabuy
ReplyDeleteRecommended Reading my link Our site learn this here now click site Full Article
ReplyDeletegiannis antetokounmpo shoes
ReplyDeletegoyard online store
cheap jordans
Travis Scott Jordan
supreme new york
golden goose
off white jordan 1
golden goose sale
off white outlet
yeezy
Free from Herpes just in 2 weeks
ReplyDeleteThe best herbal remedy
You can contact him
r.buckler11 {{@gmail}} com,, .......