In our previous blogs we have to some extent discussed sex
differences in cognitive abilities – general intelligence, specific abilities, and their relation to brain function and
structure. Here we address sex differences in creative cognition. In this area
of research, studies are not as numerous
as it is the case for intelligence. In addition to the two components usually investigated
(test scores and brain differences), researchers include yet another one, namely
real-world creative accomplishment in areas such as poetry, painting,
sculpture, music and science. The present blog will provide a brief summary of
sex differences observed in all three characteristics of creativity, providing
the most prominent theoretical explanations for the observed differences.
Sex differences in creative test performance
Given the difficulties in creativity assessment mentioned in
our previous blogs, these are even greater when trying to link them to sex differences. One of the first
review papers addressing sex differences in creativity was by Kogan (1974),
followed by Baer and Kaufman (2008), Runco et al. (2010), Pagnani (2011), and
the most recent provided by Abraham (2016). The general conclusion drawn from
these meta-analyses is that, although research results point in various
and often contradictory directions, there are probably no sex differences in
creative cognition. However, if they exists then women and girls tend to score
higher on creativity tests than men and boys. Baer and Kaufman (2008), for
instance, counted 21 studies where no sex differences in creative test
performance were observed, in three studies males scored higher than females
and in 6 studies the pattern was reverse, females outscored males. Mixed findings were reported in 17 studies. Similar findings were described
when instead of creativity tests the method of subjective assessment of
creativity was used (e.g., self-reports, or the assessment by others), or
creative personality tests were taken as measures of creativity.
Pagnani (2011) analyzed 100 published studies on sex differences
in creative potential; roughly half
of them found no such differences, while the other half produced mixed
evidence. The same inference was put forward in the most recent meta-analysis
by Abraham (2016, p. 611): “…when taken
together though, what the findings suggest is that human beings commence their
creative development on a roughly even footing, or at least on one that is not
significantly skewed solely as a function of gender.”
Some authors further suggested that mixed results in
relation to sex differences are mainly reported in studies that have focused
on comparisons of mean creativity test scores, whereas when analyzing sex
differences in variability a more clear cut picture emerged. He and Wong (2011)
for example, found support for the greater male variability hypothesis. Girls
outperformed boys in thoroughness of thinking (subscales of continuations and completitions),
while boys outperformed girls in boundary-breaking thinking (fragment dependent
and independent). The Urban & Jellen Test for Creative
Thinking-Drawing Production (TCTDP) was used in the study. A similar finding was also
reported in a study by Karwowski et al. (2016, p. 159): “Greater male variability of creative ability was found across studies
and age groups, but while this pattern was characteristic for some aspects of
creative ability (originality and unconventionality), higher female variability
was observed in the case of the other aspects (adaptiveness)”.
Sex differences in high levels of creative achievement – fine art and science
All review papers agree that sex differences in real-world
creative accomplishment are large and significant. Especially in domains such
as science, musical composition and painting far more men than women attain
eminence, whereas in expressive domains such as writing, musical performance,
dance and drama the differences are less extreme (Baer and Kaufman, 2008;
Pagnani, 2011; Abraham, 2016).
The observed sex differences have been mainly explained by
biological or socio-cultural factors. Vernon (1989, p. 102-103), for example,
argued: “It is entirely implausible that
human society should approve of females becoming highly talented performers of
music, dance, and drama, and even allowing them to become creative writers,
while, at the same time, disapproving of their becoming musical composers or
painters.” The most often mentioned biological characteristics related to sex
differences in creative performance are genetic differences, hormonal
differences, and brain differences in terms of overall size, size variability
and organization (Abrham, 2016). We have to a great extent discussed sex-related brain differences in one of our previous blogs, thus in the last section we will just focus
on those related specifically to creativity.
The genetic variability between sexes makes up 2% – 3%, and
the exposure to different hormones in early development is an important
factor in sex differentiation. An example of such an explanation is Geschwind
and Galaburda’s (1985) hypothesis that there is a relationship between
anomalous hemispheric dominance and special talents – neuropathology of
superior intellectual functions (giftedness). The higher presence of androgens
such as testosterone when a male fetus is in utero have many significant
effects on brain development which are different to that of a female fetus. Testosterone
slows the grows of the left hemisphere thereby allowing a more rapid
development of the right hemisphere. Disrupted left cortical cytoarchitecture
causes developmental learning disorders in males (dyslexia), autism, stuttering,
delayed speech, left-handedness and hyperactivity. However, the enhanced
development of the right hemisphere results in giftedness for those skills for
which the right hemisphere is particularly involved, such as mathematics
(science), music, artistic/spatial ability. Yet another explanation is the
already mentioned higher male variability (flatter normal curves), often summarized
as: "more prodigies, more idiots". Furthermore, the finding that females show more incremental (adaptive) rather
than revolutionary (innovative) creativity was linked to the evolutionary mechanisms
of sexual selection in the sense – creativity is sexy (Karwowski et al., 2016).
Kaufman et al. (2014), for instance, showed that females have a stronger
preference for ornamental/aesthetic creativity in a prospective sexual partner,
while males prefer everyday/domestic forms of creativity.
Socio-cultural explanations of sex differences in creative
performance focus on environmental and cultural influences. Most often mentioned
are historically grounded inequalities in supply of resources and support, biased
(mostly male) evaluation of creative achievements, socialization, conflicts
between family and professional roles, a lower need for achievement and the suggestion that females seem to be more
interested in the creative process than the product.
The gap in creative achievement is probably best explained
by historically grounded inequalities. For instance, at the University of
Oxford women were only allowed entry from the 1870s and were not allowed to
matriculate or graduate till 1920. The first image shows a portrait of Wolfgang and Nannerl
Mozart. Nannerl was also a gifted music performer[1]. She was part of the tour until she turned 18, then she was left behind in Salzburg,
and her father only took Wolfgang on their next journeys around the courts of
Europe. By going on tour a woman risked her reputation.
The male/female gap in creative production seems to increase after college. This was explained by the conflict of one’s own goals
and expectations of others mostly grounded in the division of culturally assigned
sex roles. It is interesting though that studies did not find a difference in
the number of published articles between married females having families and
those being single. On the other hand, females in general published less than
males (Baer and Kaufman, 2008; Pagnani, 2011; Abraham, 2016). The complexity of
the findings indicates that probably no single factor contributes to the
observed difference, but most likely the interaction between socio-cultural and
biological causes make the difference.
Sex based brain-related differences in creativity
To our knowledge, just five studies to date (two EEG and
three fMRI) have addressed sex-related differences in brain function and
structure related to creativity (Razumnikova, 2004; Fink and Neubauer, 2006;
Ryman et al., 2014; Abraham et al., 2014; Takeuchi et al., 2017).
In the study by Fink and Neubauer (2006), although no behavioral
differences in creative test performance were observed, males and females
significantly differed with respect to task-related synchronization of EEG
alpha activity in anterior regions of the cortex. Razumnikova (2004) suggested
that sex differences in creative performance were related to differences in
hemispheric brain activity.
The fMRI study by Abraham at al. (2014) also revealed no
sex-related differences in creative test performance. In contrast, the fMRI
data showed that during divergent
thinking, declarative memory related brain regions were more activated in men,
while regions involved in theory of mind and self-referential processing were
more engaged in women. The study by Ryman et al. (2014) revealed sex
differences in the relationship between global connectivity and creativity.
Females showed an inverse relationship between global connectivity and creative
cognition, whereas there were no significant relationships observed in males.
Creative females showed a more widespread activation pattern during creative test
performance, whereas creative males demonstrated more direct paths and greater clustering. The study by Takeuchi et al. (2017) revealed significant positive
correlations between regional white matter volume and creative test performance
in widespread areas below the neocortex in females.
Although the number of studies is too low for generalization, a
main finding in most of them is that similar creative test performance in both
sexes is achieved by activating different brain structures and connections,
suggesting differences in cognitive strategies employed by males and females.
This is not a new explanation for observed sex differences in cognitive performance.
Similar explanations have been put forward when analyzing the neuronal
underpinning of sex-related differences in other cognitive abilities, for example
in spatial ability (e.g., Jaušovec and Jaušovec, 2012).
References
Abraham, A.
(2016). Gender and creativity: an overview of psychological and neuroscientific
literature. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 10(2), 609–618.
https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-015-9410-8
Abraham, A.,
Thybusch, K., Pieritz, K., & Hermann, C. (2014). Gender differences in
creative thinking: behavioral and fMRI findings. Brain Imaging and Behavior,
8(1), 39–51. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-013-9241-4
Baer, J., &
Kaufman, J. C. (2008). Gender Differences in Creativity. The Journal of
Creative Behavior, 42(2), 75–105. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2162-6057.2008.tb01289.x
Fink, A., &
Neubauer, A. C. (2006). EEG alpha oscillations during the performance of verbal
creativity tasks: differential effects of sex and verbal intelligence.
International Journal of Psychophysiology, , 62(1), 46–53.
doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.01.001.
Geschwind, N.,
& Galaburda, A. M. (1985). Cerebral lateralization: Biological mechanisms,
associations, and pathology. Archives of Neurology, 42, 428-459.
He, W., &
Wong, W. (2011). Gender differences in creative thinking revisited: Findings
from analysis of variability. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(7),
807–811. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.027
Jaušovec, N.,
& Jaušovec, K. (2012). Sex differences in mental rotation and cortical
activation patterns: Can training change them? Intelligence, 40(2), 151–162.
http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.005
Karwowski, M.,
Jankowska, D. M., Gralewski, J., Gajda, A., Wiśniewska, E., & Lebuda, I.
(2016). Greater male variability in creativity: A latent variables approach.
Thinking Skills and Creativity, 22, 159–166.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2016.10.005
Kaufman, S. B.,
Kozbelt, A., Silvia, P., Kaufman, J. C., Ramesh, S., & Feist, G. J. (2016).
Who Finds Bill Gates Sexy? Creative Mate Preferences as a Function of Cognitive
Ability, Personality, and Creative Achievement. The Journal of Creative
Behavior, 50(4), 294–307. https://doi.org/10.1002/jocb.78
Kogan, N. (1974).
Creativity and sex differences. Journal of Creative Behavior, 8, 1-14.
Pagnani, A. R.
(2011). Gender differences. In M. A. Runco & S. R. Pritzker (Eds.),
Encyclopedia of creativity (Second Edition) (pp. 551–557). San Diego: Academic.
Retrieved from http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780123750389001060.
Razumnikova, O.
M. (2004). Gender differences in hemispheric organization during divergent
thinking: an EEG investigation in human subjects. Neuroscience Letters, 362(3),
193–195. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2004.02.066.
Ryman, S. G., van
den Heuvel, M. P., Yeo, R. A., Caprihan, A., Carrasco, J., Vakhtin, A. A., …
Jung, R. E. (2014). Sex differences in the relationship between white matter
connectivity and creativity. NeuroImage, 101, 380–389.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.07.027
Takeuchi, H.,
Taki, Y., Nouchi, R., Yokoyama, R., Kotozaki, Y., Nakagawa, S., … Kawashima, R.
(2017). Creative females have larger white matter structures: Evidence from a
large sample study: Creative Females Have Larger White Matter Structures. Human
Brain Mapping, 38(1), 414–430. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23369
Vernon, P. E.
(1989). The nature-nurture problem in creativity. In J. A. Glover, R. R.
Ronning, & C. R. Reynolds (Eds.), Handbook of creativity: Perspectives on
individual differences (pp. 93-110). New York: Plenum Press.
Great post! Good job.
ReplyDeleteFree from Herpes just in 2 weeks
ReplyDeleteThe best herbal remedy
You can contact him
r.buckler11 {{@gmail}} com,, .......
Mozart's gender was a product of the societal norms and expectations of the 18th century. The Perantu Panthan During that time, opportunities for women in music composition and performance were limited, and it was uncommon for women to receive formal musical education or be recognized as composers.
ReplyDelete