The cognitive
functions that predict intelligence have been most often identified as working
memory and processing speed. As stressed by Deary (2012), they represent an interim level of
reductionism between the psychometric construct of intelligence (g) and biological intelligence.
Furthermore, they provide explanations for the relation between g and activated brain areas visualized
with neuroimaging techniques (e.g., fMRI, PET), although often such
explanations do not go beyond cheap talk. In this blog we will focus on working memory
(WM). As stated by Colom et al. (2008), WM and intelligence are highly related constructs, but we
still do not know why.
Working
memory – short term memory
Working
memory relates to a system that temporarily holds or manipulates information
that we have just experienced or retrieved from long-term memory (Baddeley,
2012). The WM concept developed from an earlier one called short
term memory; both terms are still used interchangeably. As put forward by
Baddeley (2012), short-term-memory is often used in relation to its literal
meaning: keeping a limited amount of information in mind for a short period of
time. In contrast, working memory not only refers to storage of information,
but also to manipulation of this information.
Probably the
most popular and enduring conceptualization of WM is the one proposed by
Baddeley and Hitch (1974) – the multi component
model of WM. In its original form it consisted of 3 components: the central
executive and two slave systems, the visuo-spatial sketch pad and the
phonological loop. To increase the explanatory power of the model a third
storage system was introduced – the episodic buffer, a temporary interface
between short and long-term memory. In yet another updated version, the
episodic buffer received a more central position: it was still defined as a passive
system but with the crucial function of integrating information from different
sources and modalities into chunks or episodes (Baddeley, 2012).
Baddeley and Hitch (1974)
More
recently, state-based models of working memory have gained prominence
(D’Esposito & Postle, 2015). These models assume that allocation of attention to
different representations in long term memory (either semantic, sensory or
motor) governs temporary retention in working memory. The most well-known among
these models is Cowan’s embedded-processes model in which working memory is
defined as a cognitive condition that retains information in an accessible
state (Cowan et al., 2005). Activation occurs in long-term memory, is temporary, and
fades unless maintained by verbal rehearsal or continued attention. In the core
of this new theoretical framework are two constructs: focus of attention and
its capacity – scope of attention (Cowan et al., 2005).
Cowan et al. (2005)
In
state-based models as well as in the multi component model, attention is the
process that is used to explain the main functions of working memory: bringing
information from perception into the focus of attention – encoding, keeping this information in an active state – maintenance (removal of interfering
information), and bringing it back to attention when needed – retrieval (Jonides et al., 2008).
Intelligence
and WM
Psychometric
research has revealed a positive relationship between performance on tasks of
working memory and fluid intelligence, with correlations ranging from 0.60 to
0.90 (Buehner et al., 2005). Different working
memory features were suggested as being central for intelligent reasoning.
Cowan et al.
(2005) proposed that the scope-of-attention that does not include any
processing component showed rather high correlation with intelligence. In the
same direction points a large scale study by Colom et al. (2008), which
revealed that simple short term storage largely accounted for the relation
between intelligence and working memory.
In contrast,
Engle (Engle et al., 1999; Unsworth & Engle, 2007), suggested that executive functioning, especially the
control of attention over interference and conflict is the central process
contributing to the shared variance with intelligence. This attentional process is best assessed
with complex span tasks requiring participants to engage in processing activity
unrelated to the memory task. For instance in the operation span task,
participants solve math problems while trying to remember unrelated items (letters).
IS (8/2)
– 1 = 1 YES
NO A
IS (6*1)
– 2 = 4 YES
NO R
IS (8*2) – 5 = 11 YES
NO C
IS
(8/4) + 5 = 7 YES NO
D
The
respondent has to indicate if the equation is correct (yes or no) and to
remember the letters subsequently presented. When prompted, the individual is
required to recall the letters in the correct serial order (e.g., A, R, C, and
D).
Yet another
process identified as crucial for the intelligence-WM overlap was relational
integration, defined as the ability to build new relations between elements
thereby creating structural representations (Oberauer et al., 2008). For instance in the kinship task verbal descriptions of the
relationship between two people (e.g., “Anne is Barbara’s sister”, “Barbara is
Charlie’s mother”) are presented. Participants are asked to indicate the implied
relationship between two of the people mentioned in several consecutive
sentences (e.g., “Anne is Charlie’s?” the correct answer is “aunt”).
A central
problem of research into the relationship between problem solving and working
memory is the immense diversity of tasks used to measure WM. Some of these
tasks resemble those used in intelligence tests, which would mean that the
criterion is predicted by another instance of the criterion. Especially the
tasks assumed to measure relational integration have been criticized along
these lines.
Neurocognitive underpinning of WM
From a
neurocognitive perspective the fronto–parietal network has been associated with
performance on working memory tasks. It has been further suggested that the
central executive function of WM is linked to the frontal lobes, whereas the WM
storage component is associated with parietal areas (Champod & Petrides,
2010; Sauseng et al., 2010; D’Esposito & Postle, 2015). Based on evidence from
several brain imaging studies, the left intraparietal sulcus has been
identified as a unique area responsible for amodal or multimodal storage of
information (Cowan et al., 2011). Support for a fronto–parietal distinction related to the WM
functions of processing and storing of information comes also from research
employing neuroelectric brain imaging methods. Research indicates that theta
oscillations are related to working memory processes. Furthermore, there is
evidence to suggest that theta synchronizes during WM processes and serves as a
gating mechanism, providing optimal neural conditions for specific processing
(Sauseng et al., 2010).
It can be
concluded that contemporary cognitive research of working memory has favored
state-based models because they accommodate well to neuroscience data. The
focus of attention can explain the two main functions of working memory:
temporary storage of information and maintenance of stored information. The
lateral prefrontal cortex, important for attentional control represents
top-down influence on posterior sensory regions reactivating cortical memory
traces – the memories themselves (Sreenivasan et al., 2014). The similarity with neural models of intelligence is
obvious, but not unexpected given that working memory has sometimes been
proposed as virtually synonymous with intelligence (e.g., Martínez et al., 2011).
References
Baddeley, A.
D., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), The
psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory (pp.
47–89). New York: Academic press.
Baddeley, A.
(2012). Working Memory: Theories, Models, and Controversies. Annual Review of
Psychology, 63(1), 1–29. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422
Buehner, M., Krumm, S., & Pick, M.
(2005). Reasoning=working memory≠attention. Intelligence, 33(3),
251–272. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2005.01.002
Champod, A.
S., & Petrides, M. (2010). Dissociation within the Frontoparietal Network
in Verbal Working Memory: A Parametric Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Study. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(10), 3849–3856. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0097-10.2010
Colom, R.,
Abad, F. J., Quiroga, M. Á., Shih, P. C., & Flores-Mendoza, C. (2008).
Working memory and intelligence are highly related constructs, but why?
Intelligence, 36(6), 584–606. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.002
Cowan, N.,
Elliott, E. M., Scott Saults, J., Morey, C. C., Mattox, S., Hismjatullina, A.,
& Conway, A. R. A. (2005). On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and
its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cognitive Psychology,
51(1), 42–100. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2004.12.001
Cowan, N.,
Li, D., Moffitt, A., Becker, T. M., Martin, E. A., Saults, J. S., & Christ,
S. E. (2011). A neural region of abstract working memory. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 23(10), 2852–2863.
Deary, I. J.
(2012). Intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 453–482.
http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100353
D’Esposito,
M., & Postle, B. R. (2015). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Working Memory.
Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 115–142. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031
Engle, R.W.,
Tuholski, S.W., Laughlin, J.E., & Conway, A.R.A. (1999). Working memory,
shortterm memory and general fluid intelligence: A latent variable approach.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,128, 309–331.
Jonides, J.,
Lewis, R. L., Nee, D. E., Lustig, C. A., Berman, M. G., & Moore, K. S.
(2008). The mind and brain of short-term memory. Annual Review of Psychology,
59, 193–224. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093615
Martínez, K.,
Burgaleta, M., Román, F. J., Escorial, S., Shih, P. C., Quiroga, M. Á., &
Colom, R. (2011). Can fluid intelligence be reduced to “simple” short-term
storage? Intelligence, 39(6), 473–480. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2011.09.001
Oberauer, K., Süβ, H.-M., Wilhelm, O., & Wittmann, W. W.
(2008). Which working memory functions predict intelligence?
Intelligence, 36(6), 641–652. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2008.01.007
Sauseng, P., Griesmayr, B., Freunberger,
R., & Klimesch, W. (2010). Control mechanisms in working memory: A
possible function of EEG theta oscillations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral
Reviews, 34(7), 1015–1022. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.12.006
Sreenivasan,
K. K., Curtis, C. E., & D’Esposito, M. (2014). Revisiting the role of
persistent neural activity during working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
18(2), 82–89. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.001
Unsworth, N.,
and Engle, R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory
capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from
secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114, 104−132.
Good information... Having a good memory could lead to remember past information and make things perfect in your workplace without any risks.
ReplyDeleteMemory Improvement Techniquest
Concentration Exercises For Students
Kids Brain Trainer
Right Brain Training
One On One Maths Tutoring
Physics And Maths Tutor
discover this info here high replica bags our website gucci replica handbags try this web-site gucci replica bags
ReplyDeleteblog replica bags find this designer replica luggage learn the facts here now buy replica bags online
ReplyDeleter7q79r7v59 w5s45u7u51 h3q62b2z32 v6a89i7i03 f5r94y2f11 w8o60q8n64
ReplyDeleteFree from Herpes just in 2 weeks
ReplyDeleteThe best herbal remedy
You can contact him
r.buckler11 {{@gmail}} com,, .......
Working memory is a crucial component of intelligence, playing a central role in cognitive processes. How File Parse It refers to the system that temporarily holds and manipulates information necessary for complex cognitive tasks.
ReplyDelete