Comparing research on creativity training with the one on increasing
intelligence reveals several differences. However, the most apparent one is
that the idea to increase creative cognition has no real opposition as it is
the case with intelligence. Several meta-analyses showed that there is moderate
to high evidence that it is possible to raise creativity (e.g., Torrance, 1972; Scot et al., 2004; Ma, 2006). Given the importance of creativity for society
underlined in almost every research paper[1],
it is surprising that we do not face a more intense promotion and an extended
range of computerized creativity training programs, as it is the case with so-called brain training programs of intelligence. SharpBrains (2013; 2015) assessed the state of the
digital brain training market in the intelligence domain (products for assessing training-induced improvements in brain
function), reporting sales of $210 million in 2005, $600 million
in 2009, and $1.3 billion in 2013; predicting a total of $6.15 billion in
yearly sales by the year 2020 ($3.38 billion for training software). In
contrast, computerized creativity
training programs are often designed for research purposes only (e.g.,
Coppi, 2015; Fink et al., 2015). Thus we are faced with a paradox: creative
training appears to work, creativity is of immense importance for society, but there is
no broader interest/intention to boost it. Why? In our opinion the most likely
reason is the prevailing view in folk and scientific psychology that creativity
is just part of the intelligence construct. Yet another issue that hampers the development of creativity
training programs is that albeit training increases the number of original and
flexible uses for an umbrella, this has very little to do with Einstein’s
relativity theory, Mozart’s Marriage of
Figaro, Leonardo’s Mona Lisa, or Kafka’s The Trial.
Increasing creativity – behavioral level
For example, Scot et al. (2004) classified them into six
categories: (1) provisioning of
effective incentives, (2) acquisition of requisite expertise, (3) effective
structuring of group interactions, (4) optimization of climate, (5)
identification of requisite career development experiences and (6) training to
enhance creativity. It was further reported that training programs to enhance
creativity were the most preferred methods by providers from organizations as
well as educational institutions. Moreover, these training programs were predominantly
shaped by theoretical frameworks for understanding the creative act adopted by scholars
(e.g., cognition, personality,
motivation, or social interactions). The meta-analysis by Scot et al. (2004)
included 70 studies. The overall effect size obtained was 0.68 (SD = 0.09; when
corrected for outliers 0.64; SD = 0.07).
With respect to the criteria used to evaluate the training outcomes, the
highest effect sizes were obtained for divergent thinking (0.75) and problem solving (0.84), whereas for
performance and behavior criteria smaller, albeit still sizable effects were observed (0.35;
0.24). Furthermore, the highest effect sizes were obtained for
originality, followed by fluency, flexibility and elaboration. In addition it
was found that training effects were similar for older and younger individuals.
There was also no difference with respect to the institution providing the
program (educational versus organizational). The training was more beneficial
for males, and for nongifted subjects. All in all: “…. the most clear-cut conclusion to emerge from this study is that
creativity training is effective” (Scot et al., 2004; p. 381).
Even more pronounced effect sizes were reported in the meta-analysis by Ma (2006), with
a grand mean effect size of 0.77 (SD =
0.74). In this study altogether 268 effect sizes from 34 studies were included.
The training programs were divided into 10 categories with an additional category
for composite techniques. The program categories were: (1) simple ideation
program, (2) attitude training (3) synectics, (4) Idea checklist/SCAMPER (an
acronym for substitute, combine, adapt, modify/magnify/minimize, put to other
uses, eliminate, and reverse/rearrange), and (5) incubation. The programs (6) brainstorming (7) forced relation, (8)
catalog, (9) part improving, and (10) morphological synthesis were combined as “problem identifying” methods. The composite techniques included
were: Computer-aided creativity training programs, Purdue Creative Thinking
Program, New Directions in Creativity Program (based on Guilford’s SOI model); Khatena’s Training
Method, and the Osborn–Parnes CPS program. The meta-analysis further revealed
that the training effects were influenced by the moderating variables type of
training program and age of participants. The highest effect sizes were
observed for attitude training, simple
ideation and New Directions in Creativity Program (effect sizes between 0.99 to
1.46). The lowest effect sizes were obtained for the Idea checklist/SCAMPER;
incubation and the combined category problem identifying (effect sizes from
0.06 to 0.34). The effect sizes for the other programs ranged between 0.61 and 0.84. The results showed a trend that the older the age of participants, the
larger the effect size (with the exception of college students). The moderating
variables tests used to measure creativity, experimental design and duration of
training were not significant.
A Web of Science search for meta-analyses of creative
training found no more recent publications, which indicates that the interest
for these kind of programs has declined. We found only specific research highlighting particular approaches and techniques.
Colzato et al. (2012), for instance, analyzed the influence meditation
has on creativity. The authors compared the effects of Focused Attention
meditation (FA) and Open Monitoring meditation (OM) on divergent and convergent
creative thinking. During FA meditation, participants had to focus attention to
particular parts of the body while during OM meditation, they had to open the
mind to any occurring thought with a nonjudgmental attitude. Both meditation
types had specific effects on creativity. OM meditation positively influenced performance
on tasks of alternate uses that require
weak top-down guidance during generation of new ideas. In contrast, FA
meditation had no beneficial influence on the remote association test (RAT)
that would require a focused control style of operating. The authors explained
the FA effects in relation to improved mood observed after FA meditation that
could have hampered RAT performance. In yet another study, Sahin (2014) analyzed the effectiveness of mentoring showing that this method is highly effective in teaching creative
thinking skills to gifted and non-gifted students. Furthermore, Madore et al. (2015) showed that episodic-specificity induction can enhance divergent
creative thinking. Compared were episodic inductions which fostered specific recall
of events by imagery with recall of more general events without imagery. In the
former approach participants generated more categories of alternative uses than
after episodic induction without imagery. The authors inferred: “…that an episodic-specificity induction that
increases the number of episodic details participants generate in imagining
future events also boosts their performance on the AUT, a classic test of
divergent thinking” (Madore et al., 2015; p.1466).
This brief review of papers on creativity training indicates
that the topic is far from being in the mainstream of creativity research. There is a lack of elaborated general goals, recommendations
or identified trends for future exploration.
Increasing creativity – neurobiological changes
Studies aiming to relate training-induced changes in
creative performance with changes in brain function have only recently gained
in popularity, thus there is not much to review. Like in the general
research area on neurobiological underpinnings of creativity, the main lab that studies this topic is the group
involving Andreas Fink and Mathias
Benedek from the University of Graz. In one of
their first studies, Fink et al. (2010) investigated whether creative cognition can
be enhanced by idea sharing and whether the performance differences are reflected in
brain activity determined with fMRI. Respondents were exposed to two training
conditions and in one of them they were confronted with ideas (answers – alternative
uses of everyday objects) of other participants. This approach was shown to be effective
in group-based brainstorming. In the control condition the individuals were instructed to reflect on their own answers. The cognitive training was
effective in improving the originality of answers. Furthermore, the performance
improvement was also reflected in brain activity mainly in right-hemispheric
temporo-parietal, medial frontal, and posterior cingulate cortices, bilaterally.
The central finding was that creative
idea generation was associated with deactivations in parieto-temporal brain
regions.
In a second study by the same group (Jauk et al., 2015), the
influence of a 3 week verbal creativity training program on creative
performance and brain activity was investigated. Respondents were tested 3
times, and were randomly assigned into two groups. The first group received
training between the 1st and 2nd testing, whereas the second
group was trained between the 2nd and 3rd test sessions.
The training was computer based (CreaTrain)
and consisted of different verbal divergent thinking exercises requiring about
20 min of daily work. Increases in verbal creativity were only observed for the
second group receiving training between the 2nd and 3rd
testing. Contrary to behavioral results, which differed between groups, functional patterns of brain activity during
creative ideation were similar across both training groups. The main
brain areas involved were the left inferior parietal cortex and the left middle
temporal gyrus. Comparable results were also obtained in a longitudinal training study by Sun et
al., (2016) who used a similar training paradigm as the one adopted by Fink et
al. (2010). In addition to the functional changes in brain activity, the training
induced gray matter volume increases in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex.
In summary, it seems that creative training works and shows
also changes in brain activity and structure, albeit the latter conclusion must
be taken with care given the limited number of studies it is based on. All authors agreed
that creative training changed brain activation patterns in exactly those brain
areas that had been associated with creativity in correlational studies. This
is of course not surprising since creativity was shown to be related to almost every brain area –
either activating or deactivating it, thereby limiting the conclusions that can be drawn regarding the influence of creative training on brain activity.
A question that remains is: to what extent are these
creativity training programs similar to the criterion tests? For several of
them we could say that the improvements point to near transfer effects, such
as improvements in n-back performance after n-back training. What about far transfer effects?
To exaggerate, do you become a poet or a novelist after verbal ideational
training of the CreaTrain type? Perhaps the answer NO is one of the reasons why there is a lack of interest
in this kind of research.
References
Colzato, L. S., Ozturk, A., & Hommel, B. (2012).
Meditate to Create: The Impact of Focused-Attention and Open-Monitoring
Training on Convergent and Divergent Thinking. Frontiers in Psychology, 3. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00116
Coppi, A. E.
(2015). Fostering Creativity through Games and Digital Story Telling
(pp. 17–21). IEEE. https://doi.org/10.1109/iTAG.2015.12
Fink, A.,
Benedek, M., Koschutnig, K., Pirker, E., Berger, E., Meister, S., … Weiss,
E. M. (2015). Training of verbal creativity modulates brain activity in regions
associated with language- and memory-related demands: Training of Verbal
Creativity. Human Brain Mapping,
36(10), 4104–4115. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22901
Fink, A.,
Grabner, R. H., Gebauer, D., Reishofer, G., Koschutnig, K., & Ebner, F.
(2010). Enhancing creativity by means of cognitive stimulation: Evidence
from an fMRI study. NeuroImage, 52(4), 1687–1695.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.05.072
Ma, H.-H. (2006). A synthetic analysis of the effectiveness
of single components and packages in creativity training programs. Creativity
Research Journal, 18(4), 435–446.
Madore, K. P., Addis, D. R., & Schacter, D. L. (2015).
Creativity and memory effects of an episodic-specificity induction on divergent
thinking. Psychological Science, 956797615591863.
Şahin, F. (2014). The effectiveness of mentoring strategy
for developing the creative potential of the gifted and non-gifted students.
Thinking Skills and Creativity, 14, 47–55.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tsc.2014.07.002
Scott, G., Leritz, L. E., & Mumford, M. D. (2004). The
effectiveness of creativity training: A quantitative review. Creativity
Research Journal, 16(4), 361–388.
SharpBrains. (2013). Web-based, mobile and biometrics-based
technology to assess, monitor and enhance cognition and brain functioning: The
digital brain health market, 2012–2020. San Francisco, CA: SharpBrains.
SharpBrains. (2015, January). The digital brain health
market 2012–2020: Web-based, mobile and biometrics-based 184 technology to
assess, monitor, and enhance cognition and brain functioning [Addendum]. San
Francisco, CA: SharpBrains.
Sun, J., Chen, Q., Zhang, Q., Li, Y., Li, H., Wei, D., …
Qiu, J. (2016). Training your brain to be more creative: brain functional and
structural changes induced by divergent thinking training: The Neural
Plasticity of Creativity. Human Brain Mapping, 37(10), 3375–3387.
https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23246
Torrance, E. P. (1972). Can we teach children to think
creatively? Journal of Creative Behavior, 6, 114–143.
[1]Importance for entrepreneurial activities
and long-term economic growth; linked to the development of new social
institutions,“good” jobs, well-being and successful adaptation to the demands
of daily life; the evolution of civilization;
creativity can help us understand the rapidly increasing complexity of the world
around us, where it is estimated that human knowledge doubles every seven
years, etc.
Free from Herpes just in 2 weeks
ReplyDeleteThe best herbal remedy
You can contact him
r.buckler11 {{@gmail}} com,, .......
Very thoughtfful blog
ReplyDelete